Skip to main content

Words with pictures: help or hindrance?

First, I'm going to assume that most of you do teach vocabulary explicitly to novices and that you often do so with pictures. There are good reasons for doing so - memorability, avoiding interference from the first language, holding pupils' interest to name three.

When you show pictures to beginners or near-beginners on a PowerPoint or hand-held flashcard do you prefer to show the written word with the picture or not? At what point do you like pupils to see the written word? Here are a few thoughts on the issue.

Now when I began teaching I was encouraged (in the good old direct method - avoid English - way) to introduce spoken words before written ones. The rationale was something like this:

  • When we learn a first language in early years we do so almost solely without the use of written words. If we believe second language acquisition is fundamentally the same as fist language acquisition, why not try to match the way caregivers "teach" their children?
  • Speech is primary, writing secondary. Our brains are "wired" to acquire language orally. Teach sounds first, written words later.
  • Seeing the written word can lead many pupils to mispronounce because of interference from the first language, so avoid showing the written word until pronunciation is accurate.
  • Not seeing the word may get pupils to fully enjoy the phonology of the language without distraction from orthography.
I largely stuck to this approach throughout my career, but became gradually just a little less enamoured with it. I became a tad less ideological and a tad more pragmatic, if you like.

The recent focus on phonics (matching phonemes and syllables to graphemes) has meant that more teachers these days may be keen to show pupils the equivalence between oral and written language at an earlier stage. This might mean, for example, displaying the word alongside the picture immediately and highlighting particular letter combinations, particularly ones which might cause difficulty (e.g. in French "oi" "au" "ou" "u" "er" and so on). The rationale for showing the written word from the outset might go something like this:
  • Seeing the word and the picture together might lead to better retention.
  • Pupils quickly learn sound-letter matches so become better readers and pronouncers in the long run (e.g. they might be able to pronounce nonsense words accurately as they do in those L1 primary school tests).
  • Seeing spellings might remind pupils about cognates and similarities or differences in spelling between the target language and English.
  • Seeing the word early on may avoid subsequent spelling errors (as pupils guess the spelling from the sounds they have heard (e.g. in French 5 = sank).
I'm not sure there are any right answers here and if I had to recommend anything it would be to delay showing the word or phrase initially to try to establish good pronunciation, then quickly show the spelling soon after. Get pupils to repeat both from the sounds alone AND by reading the written word aloud. This allows you to have another pass at a picture with an added element, thus providing more repetition in a slightly different way (generally a smart language teaching approach). To reiterate, we don't really want pupils to guess spellings, but we also want them to repeat sounds as accurately as possible.

Some readers may prefer to avoid teaching vocabulary in this way at all, e.g. by not using pictures or always teaching words in the context of sentences or short paragraphs. Some may even believe that vocab is best learned implicitly through general listening and reading, but research (e.g. the vocabulary "gurus" Paul Nation and Batia Laufer) supports explicit teaching of words and chunks to both beginners and, to a lesser extent, more experienced learners.

I wonder what you think. Feel free to comment!

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

What is the natural order hypothesis?

The natural order hypothesis states that all learners acquire the grammatical structures of a language in roughly the same order. This applies to both first and second language acquisition. This order is not dependent on the ease with which a particular language feature can be taught; in English, some features, such as third-person "-s" ("he runs") are easy to teach in a classroom setting, but are not typically fully acquired until the later stages of language acquisition. The hypothesis was based on morpheme studies by Heidi Dulay and Marina Burt, which found that certain morphemes were predictably learned before others during the course of second language acquisition. The hypothesis was picked up by Stephen Krashen who incorporated it in his very well known input model of second language learning. Furthermore, according to the natural order hypothesis, the order of acquisition remains the same regardless of the teacher's explicit instruction; in other words,

What is skill acquisition theory?

For this post, I am drawing on a section from the excellent book by Rod Ellis and Natsuko Shintani called Exploring Language Pedagogy through Second Language Acquisition Research (Routledge, 2014). Skill acquisition is one of several competing theories of how we learn new languages. It’s a theory based on the idea that skilled behaviour in any area can become routinised and even automatic under certain conditions through repeated pairing of stimuli and responses. When put like that, it looks a bit like the behaviourist view of stimulus-response learning which went out of fashion from the late 1950s. Skill acquisition draws on John Anderson’s ACT theory, which he called a cognitivist stimulus-response theory. ACT stands for Adaptive Control of Thought.  ACT theory distinguishes declarative knowledge (knowledge of facts and concepts, such as the fact that adjectives agree) from procedural knowledge (knowing how to do things in certain situations, such as understand and speak a language).

12 principles of second language teaching

This is a short, adapted extract from our book The Language Teacher Toolkit . "We could not possibly recommend a single overall method for second language teaching, but the growing body of research we now have points to certain provisional broad principles which might guide teachers. Canadian professors Patsy Lightbown and Nina Spada (2013), after reviewing a number of studies over the years to see whether it is better to just use meaning-based approaches or to include elements of explicit grammar teaching and practice, conclude: Classroom data from a number of studies offer support for the view that form-focused instruction and corrective feedback provided within the context of communicative and content-based programmes are more effective in promoting second language learning than programmes that are limited to a virtually exclusive emphasis on comprehension. As teachers Gianfranco and I would go along with that general view and would like to suggest our own set of g