Skip to main content

La Renault Zoé

Renault vient de lancer sa petite voiture électrique, la Zoé. Ce qui rend cette nouvelle née plus attractive que les autres électriques, c'est le prix: £13650 (plus £70 par mois pour les batteries avec service de dépannage en cas de batterie morte). La Nissan Leaf par exemple coûte le double.

Image: caradisiac.com
Selon les magazines de l'automobile c'est une voiture performante, bien équipée, mais qui a une autonomie limitée de 220 km (ou bien moins par temps froid ou en ville). Il faut entre 30 minutes et 9 heures pour recharger les batteries. Les avantages pour l'environnement sont évidentes. Les émissions de CO2 varient d'un pays à l'autre suivant l'origine de l'électricité. Avec la forte part de nucléaire, elles sont de 12g/km de CO2 en France. En Angleterre ce chiffre pourrait être vers les 60g/km, mais tout dépend du type de trajet effectué.

Pour celui ou celle qui ne fait que des petits trajets quotidiens et qui veut faire réduire ses émissions de CO2, la Zoé semble être une solution idéale. Renault espère en vendre plusieurs dizaines de milliers d'exemplaires chaque année. La Zoé sera disponible en Angleterre à partir de l'automne.

On se demande combien il va falloir augmenter la production future de l'électricité pour faire fonctionner toutes ces voitures électriques.

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

What is the natural order hypothesis?

The natural order hypothesis states that all learners acquire the grammatical structures of a language in roughly the same order. This applies to both first and second language acquisition. This order is not dependent on the ease with which a particular language feature can be taught; in English, some features, such as third-person "-s" ("he runs") are easy to teach in a classroom setting, but are not typically fully acquired until the later stages of language acquisition. The hypothesis was based on morpheme studies by Heidi Dulay and Marina Burt, which found that certain morphemes were predictably learned before others during the course of second language acquisition. The hypothesis was picked up by Stephen Krashen who incorporated it in his very well known input model of second language learning. Furthermore, according to the natural order hypothesis, the order of acquisition remains the same regardless of the teacher's explicit instruction; in other words,

What is skill acquisition theory?

For this post, I am drawing on a section from the excellent book by Rod Ellis and Natsuko Shintani called Exploring Language Pedagogy through Second Language Acquisition Research (Routledge, 2014). Skill acquisition is one of several competing theories of how we learn new languages. It’s a theory based on the idea that skilled behaviour in any area can become routinised and even automatic under certain conditions through repeated pairing of stimuli and responses. When put like that, it looks a bit like the behaviourist view of stimulus-response learning which went out of fashion from the late 1950s. Skill acquisition draws on John Anderson’s ACT theory, which he called a cognitivist stimulus-response theory. ACT stands for Adaptive Control of Thought.  ACT theory distinguishes declarative knowledge (knowledge of facts and concepts, such as the fact that adjectives agree) from procedural knowledge (knowing how to do things in certain situations, such as understand and speak a language).

12 principles of second language teaching

This is a short, adapted extract from our book The Language Teacher Toolkit . "We could not possibly recommend a single overall method for second language teaching, but the growing body of research we now have points to certain provisional broad principles which might guide teachers. Canadian professors Patsy Lightbown and Nina Spada (2013), after reviewing a number of studies over the years to see whether it is better to just use meaning-based approaches or to include elements of explicit grammar teaching and practice, conclude: Classroom data from a number of studies offer support for the view that form-focused instruction and corrective feedback provided within the context of communicative and content-based programmes are more effective in promoting second language learning than programmes that are limited to a virtually exclusive emphasis on comprehension. As teachers Gianfranco and I would go along with that general view and would like to suggest our own set of g